Warning: foreach() argument must be of type array|object, bool given in /var/www/html/web/app/themes/studypress-core-theme/template-parts/header/mobile-offcanvas.php on line 20

We are conducting many hypothesis tests to test a claim. In every case, assume that the null hypothesis is true. Approximately how many of the tests will incorrectly find significance? 100 tests conducted using a significance level of \(5 \%\).

Short Answer

Expert verified
If null hypothesis is true in every case and 100 tests are conducted with \(5\%\) significance level, we would incorrectly find significance in nearly 5 tests.

Step by step solution

01

Understand Type I error and significance level

Firstly, understand the concept of Type I error and significance level in hypothesis testing. A Type I error occurs when a true null hypothesis is rejected, and the significance level of a test is the probability of committing a Type I error. It is represented by the Greek letter alpha (\(\alpha\)).
02

Apply significance level to calculate number of incorrect tests

Given that a significance level of \(5\%\) is used, this means that if the null hypothesis is true, we may incorrectly reject it \(5\%\) of the time simply due to sample variability. Applying this to the 100 tests conducted, we multiply the number of tests by the significance level to find the number of tests that we would expect to incorrectly find significance: \(100 \times 5\% = 5\).
03

Provide final answer

Therefore, if the null hypothesis is true in every case and we perform 100 tests with a significance level of \(5\%\), we would expect to incorrectly reject the null hypothesis in approximately 5 tests.

Unlock Step-by-Step Solutions & Ace Your Exams!

  • Full Textbook Solutions

    Get detailed explanations and key concepts

  • Unlimited Al creation

    Al flashcards, explanations, exams and more...

  • Ads-free access

    To over 500 millions flashcards

  • Money-back guarantee

    We refund you if you fail your exam.

Over 30 million students worldwide already upgrade their learning with Vaia!

Key Concepts

These are the key concepts you need to understand to accurately answer the question.

Type I Error
When we talk about a Type I error in the realm of statistics, we're addressing a specific kind of mistake. Imagine you've taken a buzzer to respond to true or false questions, and even though the statement is true, you hit false. This is analogous to a Type I error, where a correct null hypothesis is wrongly rejected.

In a statistical hypothesis test, the null hypothesis, denoted as H0, is essentially our default assumption. It's the status quo, such as 'this medicine has no effect' or 'the new program doesn't change test scores'. When we perform a test, we're seeing if the data provides enough evidence to dismiss this assumption. However, there's always a chance of getting misled by random chance, causing us to think we found proof when there was none – this is the notorious Type I error.

Referring to the exercise, with 100 hypothesis tests each at a 5% significance level, our expectation is that about 5 would lead us down the wrong path, giving us a false signal of significance when there's really nothing there. It's a reminder of the fallibility of statistical conclusions – they always come with a degree of uncertainty.
Significance Level
The significance level, typically denoted by \( \alpha \), is the threshold we set to determine when to declare statistical significance. Think of it like setting a filter on your email to catch spam – the filter catches most spam, but occasionally, a genuine message slips into the junk folder. The significance level is like the sensitivity of this filter, controlling our cautiousness in labeling results 'significant'.

Generally, a 5% significance level, or \( \alpha = 0.05 \), is the conventional standard. This means we're willing to accept a 5% chance of making a Type I error – of mistakenly hitting the buzzer for false. In the exercise, with 100 tests at this \( \alpha \) level, the math is straightforward. It's like planning that out of 100 emails, you're okay with incorrectly sending 5 to spam. It provides a balance, allowing for meaningful detection of effects while controlling the rate at which we make these Type I errors.
Null Hypothesis
At the core of any hypothesis test lies the null hypothesis (H0), which is the skeptic's starting point. It represents the statement or condition that indicates no effect or no difference. Suppose we're back in school, and there's a rumor that a new teaching method will improve grades. The null hypothesis is the equivalent of saying, 'This method won't make any difference.' It's what we aim to challenge with our data.

In the exercise scenario, the null hypothesis is assumed to be true for all 100 tests. This serves as a baseline from which we measure any deviation as either due to a true effect or simply random chance. Whenever we conduct a test, we seek evidence strong enough to cast doubt on the null hypothesis. If found, we can reject it in favor of an alternative hypothesis, which might claim, 'Yes, this method does boost grades.' But herein lies the possibility of committing a Type I error, mistaking randomness for actual evidence, as discussed earlier.
Statistical Significance
Have you ever experienced a moment when you're certain something special is happening, like when a shy friend speaks up and you think, ‘This is significant’? In statistics, we’re also seeking to identify such moments, but we need more than a hunch – we require evidence. Statistical significance is the conclusion that an observed effect is unlikely to be due to chance alone.

This is determined by p-values and the pre-determined significance level, with the widespread benchmark of 0.05 setting the standard for claiming significance. To say a result is statistically significant is to assert we have enough evidence to believe something noteworthy is occurring. In the context of our exercise, when we reach a significant result, it should mean we're at least 95% confident there's a real effect at play. However, it's vital to remember that statistical significance doesn't mean certainty – there's always room for a small proportion of those surprises, the Type I errors we're keen to keep in check.

One App. One Place for Learning.

All the tools & learning materials you need for study success - in one app.

Get started for free

Most popular questions from this chapter

Scientists studying lion attacks on humans in Tanzania \(^{32}\) found that 95 lion attacks happened between \(6 \mathrm{pm}\) and \(10 \mathrm{pm}\) within either five days before a full moon or five days after a full moon. Of these, 71 happened during the five days after the full moon while the other 24 happened during the five days before the full moon. Does this sample of lion attacks provide evidence that attacks are more likely after a full moon? In other words, is there evidence that attacks are not equally split between the two five-day periods? Use StatKey or other technology to find the p-value, and be sure to show all details of the test. (Note that this is a test for a single proportion since the data come from one sample.)

Translating Information to Other Significance Levels Suppose in a two-tailed test of \(H_{0}: \rho=0\) vs \(H_{a}: \rho \neq 0,\) we reject \(H_{0}\) when using a \(5 \%\) significance level. Which of the conclusions below (if any) would also definitely be valid for the same data? Explain your reasoning in each case. (a) Reject \(H_{0}: \rho=0\) in favor of \(H_{a}: \rho \neq 0\) at a \(1 \%\) significance level. (b) Reject \(H_{0}: \rho=0\) in favor of \(H_{a}: \rho \neq 0\) at a \(10 \%\) significance level. (c) Reject \(H_{0}: \rho=0\) in favor of the one-tail alternative, \(H_{a}: \rho>0,\) at a \(5 \%\) significance level, assuming the sample correlation is positive.

Do you think that students undergo physiological changes when in potentially stressful situations such as taking a quiz or exam? A sample of statistics students were interrupted in the middle of a quiz and asked to record their pulse rates (beats for a 1-minute period). Ten of the students had also measured their pulse rate while sitting in class listening to a lecture, and these values were matched with their quiz pulse rates. The data appear in Table 4.18 and are stored in QuizPulse10. Note that this is paired data since we have two values, a quiz and a lecture pulse rate, for each student in the sample. The question of interest is whether quiz pulse rates tend to be higher, on average, than lecture pulse rates. (Hint: Since this is paired data, we work with the differences in pulse rate for each student between quiz and lecture. If the differences are \(D=\) quiz pulse rate minus lecture pulse rate, the question of interest is whether \(\mu_{D}\) is greater than zero.) Table 4.18 Quiz and Lecture pulse rates for I0 students $$\begin{array}{lcccccccccc} \text { Student } & 1 & 2 & 3 & 4 & 5 & 6 & 7 & 8 & 9 & 10 \\ \hline \text { Quiz } & 75 & 52 & 52 & 80 & 56 & 90 & 76 & 71 & 70 & 66 \\\ \text { Lecture } & 73 & 53 & 47 & 88 & 55 & 70 & 61 & 75 & 61 & 78 \\\\\hline\end{array}$$ (a) Define the parameter(s) of interest and state the null and alternative hypotheses. (b) Determine an appropriate statistic to measure and compute its value for the original sample. (c) Describe a method to generate randomization samples that is consistent with the null hypothesis and reflects the paired nature of the data. There are several viable methods. You might use shuffled index cards, a coin, or some other randomization procedure. (d) Carry out your procedure to generate one randomization sample and compute the statistic you chose in part (b) for this sample. (e) Is the statistic for your randomization sample more extreme (in the direction of the alternative) than the original sample?

Euchre One of the authors and some statistician friends have an ongoing series of Euchre games that will stop when one of the two teams is deemed to be statistically significantly better than the other team. Euchre is a card game and each game results in a win for one team and a loss for the other. Only two teams are competing in this series, which we'll call team A and team B. (a) Define the parameter(s) of interest. (b) What are the null and alternative hypotheses if the goal is to determine if either team is statistically significantly better than the other at winning Euchre? (c) What sample statistic(s) would they need to measure as the games go on? (d) Could the winner be determined after one or two games? Why or why not? (e) Which significance level, \(5 \%\) or \(1 \%,\) will make the game last longer?

Do iPads Help Kindergartners Learn: A Series of Tests Exercise 4.147 introduces a study in which half of the kindergarten classes in a school district are randomly assigned to receive iPads. We learn that the results are significant at the \(5 \%\) level (the mean for the iPad group is significantly higher than for the control group) for the results on the HRSIW subtest. In fact, the HRSIW subtest was one of 10 subtests and the results were not significant for the other 9 tests. Explain, using the problem of multiple tests, why we might want to hesitate before we run out to buy iPads for all kindergartners based on the results of this study.

See all solutions

Recommended explanations on Math Textbooks

View all explanations

What do you think about this solution?

We value your feedback to improve our textbook solutions.

Study anywhere. Anytime. Across all devices.

Sign-up for free