Warning: foreach() argument must be of type array|object, bool given in /var/www/html/web/app/themes/studypress-core-theme/template-parts/header/mobile-offcanvas.php on line 20

Are very young infants more likely to imitate actions that are modeled by a person or simulated by an object? This question was the basis of a research study summarized in the article "The Role of Person and Object in Eliciting Early Imitation" (Journal of Experimental Child Psychology [1991]: 423-433). One action examined was mouth opening. This action was modeled repeatedly by either a person or a doll, and the number of times that the infant imitated the behavior was recorded. Twentyseven infants participated, with 12 exposed to a human model and 15 exposed to the doll. Summary values are given here. Is there sufficient evidence to conclude that the mean number of imitations is higher for infants who watch a human model than for infants who watch a doll? Test the relevant hypotheses using a .01 significance level.

Short Answer

Expert verified
The solution can be reached by conducting a one-tailed t-test. However, specific calculations cannot be made without the actual data. If the p-value obtained is less than the significance level of 0.01, then there is sufficient evidence to accept the alternative hypothesis that infants are more likely to imitate actions modeled by a human than a doll.

Step by step solution

01

Formulating Hypotheses

The null hypothesis (\(H_0\)) is that there is no difference in the mean number of imitations of very young infants who watch a human model than those who watch a doll; mathematically, \(μ1 = μ2\). The Alternative hypothesis (\(H_1\)) is that the mean number of imitations is higher for infants who watch a human model than for infants who watch a doll; mathematically, \(μ1 > μ2\). Here, \(μ1\) and \(μ2\) represent the population means of imitations for infants who watched a human and a doll respectively.
02

Conducting t-test

Since the problem does not provide specific data, we can represent the conducting t-test step generally. To perform the t-test, use the given values of the results from both groups in the formula for the t-statistic \(t_{observed} = \frac{\bar{x}_1 - \bar{x}_2}{√{(s_1^2/n_1 + s_2^2/n_2)}}\), where \(\bar{x}_1\) and \(\bar{x}_2\) are the sample means of the human model and doll groups respectively, \(s_1^2\) and \(s_2^2\) are the sample variances of the two groups respectively, and \(n_1\) and \(n_2\) are the sample sizes of the two groups respectively. This will give us the observed t-statistic value.
03

Calculating Degrees of Freedom

Degrees of freedom is calculated by adding the number of items in each group and subtracting 2 (\(df = n_1 + n_2 - 2\)). The degree of freedom is used to determine the critical value from the t-distribution table.
04

Determining the p-value

The p-value is the probability that the results of the experiment occurred by chance. Using the calculated t-statistic and degree of freedom, look up the corresponding p-value in the t-distribution table.
05

Making the Decision

If the p-value is less than the significance level (0.01 in this case), then it can be concluded that the null hypothesis can be rejected and there is sufficient evidence to say that the mean number of imitations by infants who viewed a human model is greater than those who watched a doll.

Unlock Step-by-Step Solutions & Ace Your Exams!

  • Full Textbook Solutions

    Get detailed explanations and key concepts

  • Unlimited Al creation

    Al flashcards, explanations, exams and more...

  • Ads-free access

    To over 500 millions flashcards

  • Money-back guarantee

    We refund you if you fail your exam.

Over 30 million students worldwide already upgrade their learning with Vaia!

Key Concepts

These are the key concepts you need to understand to accurately answer the question.

Null and Alternative Hypotheses
Understanding the null and alternative hypotheses is crucial in hypothesis testing. The null hypothesis, denoted as \(H_0\), represents a statement of no effect or no difference. It is the default assumption that there is no relationship between two measured phenomena. In the context of our exercise, the null hypothesis suggests that the mean number of imitations by infants is the same whether they observed a human or a doll, mathematically stated as \(μ1 = μ2\).

The alternative hypothesis, denoted as \(H_1\) or \(H_a\), is a statement that contradicts the null hypothesis. It posits there is an effect or a difference. For the given exercise, the alternative hypothesis claims that infants who observed a human model have a higher mean number of imitations compared to those who observed a doll, formally written as \(μ1 > μ2\). This hypothesis is directional, as it specifies that one mean is greater than the other, rather than simply being different.

When we perform hypothesis testing, we aim to collect evidence to support the alternative hypothesis while simultaneously evaluating the likelihood of the null hypothesis being true. This sets the stage for employing statistical tools to make an informed decision based on data.
T-test Statistical Analysis
The t-test is a statistical analysis used to determine if there is a significant difference between the means of two groups, which may be related in certain features. The t-test takes into account the sample size and variation within the data to help researchers decide if their findings can be generalized to the larger population.

In our exercise, to compare the mean number of imitations between infants exposed to human models and those exposed to dolls, we would use an independent t-test. This test is appropriate when comparing two independent groups, as in our case with two distinct sets of infants. The formula to calculate the observed t-value is: \(t_{observed} = \frac{\bar{x}_1 - \bar{x}_2}{\sqrt{(s_1^2/n_1 + s_2^2/n_2)}}\), where \(

\bar{x}_1

\) and \(

\bar{x}_2

\) are the sample means, \(

s_1^2

\) and \(

s_2^2

\) are the sample variances, and \(n_1\) and \(n_2\) are the sample sizes of the human and doll groups, respectively.

The t-test analyzes the size of the difference between groups relative to the variation in the data. A high t-value can indicate a significant difference between group means if the value is higher than what would be expected by random chance.
P-value Significance
The p-value is a fundamental concept in statistical hypothesis testing, offering a measure of the strength of evidence against the null hypothesis. It helps researchers decide whether their results are statistically significant.

The p-value is the probability of obtaining test results at least as extreme as the ones observed during the test, assuming that the null hypothesis is true. So, a low p-value indicates that the observed data are unlikely under the null hypothesis. In the context of our exercise, if the p-value is less than the chosen significance level (0.01, indicating a 1% threshold for declaring a finding statistically significant), we reject the null hypothesis. This would suggest that there is a significant difference in the mean number of imitations between infants exposed to human models and those exposed to dolls.

Researchers typically set a threshold before the study, known as the alpha level (\(α\)), to determine the cut-off for significance. Common alpha levels are 0.05, 0.01, or 0.001. When we say that a result is statistically significant at the 0.01 level, we are confidently stating that there is only a 1% chance that the result is due to random variation and not because of the factor we are testing.
Degrees of Freedom Calculation
Degrees of freedom (df) are essential in the context of t-tests, as they are used to determine the critical values from the t-distribution, which then allow us to calculate the p-value.

The degrees of freedom can be thought of as the number of independent pieces of information that go into the estimation of a parameter. When conducting an independent t-test, as in our infant imitation study, the degrees of freedom are calculated based on the sample size of the two groups being compared. The formula for an independent t-test is \( df = n_1 + n_2 - 2 \), where \(n_1\) and \(n_2\) are the sample sizes of the two groups.

Once the degrees of freedom are calculated, we use this value to reference the correct distribution — the t-distribution — which helps in finding the critical value that corresponds to our significance level. By comparing the observed t-value to this critical value, or by using the degrees of freedom to get the p-value from a t-distribution table or software, we determine whether the results are statistically significant. Thus, degrees of freedom not only influence the shape of the t-distribution but also the accuracy of our significance testing.

One App. One Place for Learning.

All the tools & learning materials you need for study success - in one app.

Get started for free

Most popular questions from this chapter

Are college students who take a freshman orientation course more or less likely to stay in college than those who do not take such a course? The article "A Longitudinal Study of the Retention and Academic Performance of Participants in Freshmen Orientation Courses" (Journal of College Student Development \([1994]: 444-\) 449) reported that 50 of 94 randomly selected students who did not participate in an orientation course returned for a second year. Of 94 randomly selected students who did take the orientation course, 56 returned for a second year. Construct a \(95 \%\) confidence interval for \(\pi_{1}-\pi_{2}\), the difference in the proportion returning for students who do not take an orientation course and those who do. Give an interpretation of this interval.

Here's one to sink your teeth into: The authors of the article "Analysis of Food Crushing Sounds During Mastication: Total Sound Level Studies" (Journal of Texture Studies \([1990]: 165-178\) ) studied the nature of sound: generated during eating. Peak loudness (in decibels at \(20 \mathrm{~cm}\) away) was measured for both open-mouth and closed-mouth chewing of potato chips and of tortilla chips Forty subjects participated, with ten assigned at random te each combination of conditions (such as closed-mouth, potato chip, and so on). We are not making this up! Summary values taken from plots given in the article appear in the accompanying table. \begin{tabular}{lccc} & \(n\) & \(\bar{x}\) & \(s\) \\ \hline Potato Chip & & & \\ Open mouth & 10 & 63 & 13 \\ Closed mouth & 10 & 54 & 16 \\ Tortilla Chip & & & \\ Open mouth & 10 & 60 & 15 \\ Closed mouth & 10 & 53 & 16 \\ & & & \\ \hline \end{tabular} a. Construct a \(95 \%\) confidence interval for the difference in mean peak loudness between open-mouth and closed-

The article "Spray Flu Vaccine May Work Better Than Injections for Tots" (San Luis Obispo Tribune, May 2,2006 ) described a study that compared flu vaccine administered by injection and flu vaccine administered as a nasal spray. Each of the 8000 children under the age of 5 that participated in the study received both a nasal spray and an injection, but only one was the real vaccine and the other was salt water. At the end of the flu season, it was determined that \(3.9 \%\) of the 4000 children receiving the real vaccine by nasal spray got sick with the flu and \(8.6 \%\) of the 4000 receiving the real vaccine by injection got sick with the flu. a. Why would the researchers give every child both a nasal spray and an injection? b. Use the given data to estimate the difference in the proportion of children who get sick with the flu after being vaccinated with an injection and the proportion of children who get sick with the flu after being vaccinated with the nasal spray using a \(99 \%\) confidence interval. Based on the confidence interval, would you conclude that the proportion of children who get the flu is different for the two vaccination methods?

Do teenage boys worry more than teenage girls? This is one of the questions addressed by the authors of the article "The Relationship of Self-Esteem and Attributional Style to Young People's Worries" (Journal of Psychology [1987]: 207-215). A scale called the Worries Scale was administered to a group of teenagers, and the results are summarized in the accompanying table. \begin{tabular}{lccc} & & Sample & Sample \\ Gender & \(n\) & Mean Score & sd \\ \hline Girls & 108 & \(62.05\) & \(9.5\) \\ Boys & 78 & \(67.59\) & \(9.7\) \\ & & & \\ \hline \end{tabular} Is there sufficient evidence to conclude that teenage boys score higher on the Worries Scale than teenage girls? Use a significance level of \(\alpha=.05\).

The authors of the paper "Inadequate Physician Knowledge of the Effects of Diet on Blood Lipids and Lipoproteins" (Nutrition Journal [2003]: 19-26) summarize the responses to a questionnaire on basic knowledge of nutrition that was mailed to 6000 physicians selected at random from a list of physicians licensed in the United States. Sixteen percent of those who received the questionnaire completed and returned it. The authors report that 26 of 120 cardiologists and 222 of 419 internists did not know that carbohydrate was the diet component most likely to raise triglycerides. a. Estimate the difference between the proportion of cardiologists and the proportion of internists who did not know that carbohydrate was the diet component most likely to raise triglycerides using a \(95 \%\) confidence interval. b. What potential source of bias might limit your ability to generalize the estimate from Part (a) to the populations of all cardiologists and all internists?

See all solutions

Recommended explanations on Math Textbooks

View all explanations

What do you think about this solution?

We value your feedback to improve our textbook solutions.

Study anywhere. Anytime. Across all devices.

Sign-up for free